How Negative Interest Rates Pervert Investment Decisions

Of all the disruption inflicted on the capital markets, negative interest rates cause the most head scratching. Why would anyone invest with the certainty of a loss? New research explains the perverse behavior induced by negative rates – and offers a stern warning for investors.

The bear market caused by the coronavirus led the Federal Reserve to rapidly lower the Fed funds rate to effectively zero. The flight to quality and liquidity led to yields on short-term Treasury bills to turn negative. For example, on March 25, 2020, the yield on three-month Treasury bills fell to -0.08%. That means that investors buying a three-month Treasury bill and holding it until maturity will, with certainty, experience a loss of 0.08%. If yields on very short-term Treasury securities remain negative, as portfolios of money market funds owning only those securities “roll over” (older bills mature and new ones are added), their net asset value will eventually fall below $1 as losses are incurred.

Given that this would be a “breaking of the buck,” let’s consider how investors will react. Will the negative yields lead to investors increase risks in their portfolios in order to avoid a certain loss?

Behavioral finance

The field of behavioral finance provides us with fascinating insights into individual investor behavior, including how individuals view risk, as well as the impact of those views on asset prices. Prospect theory plays a major role in explaining investor behavior. The theory, formulated in 1979 by Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman, describes how individuals make choices between probabilistic alternatives where risk is involved and the probability of different outcomes is unknown. Prospect theory, which aims to describe the actual behavior of people, challenges the conventional “expected utility” theory, which models the decision that perfectly rational agents would make.

Prospect theory starts with the concept of loss aversion – the observation that people react differently to potential gains than to potential losses – and make decisions based on the potential gain or loss relative to their specific situation rather than in absolute terms. Faced with a risky choice leading to gains, individuals are risk-averse, preferring solutions that lead to a lower expected utility but with a higher certainty (a concave value function). On the other hand, faced with a risky choice leading to losses, individuals are loss-averse (risk-seeking), preferring solutions that lead to a lower expected utility as long as it has the potential to avoid losses (a convex value function). Those examples contradict expected utility theory, which assume choices are made to maximize utility. This is also the explanation for the “disposition effect,” whereby people would rather hold on to a stock and expose themselves to a greater loss than realize the paper loss, accepting a shortfall from their aspiration (reference point) to break-even (referred to as shortfall aversion).

Maren Baars, Henning Cordes and Hannes Mohrschladt, authors of the study, How Negative Interest Rates Affect the Risk-Taking of Individual Investors: Experimental Evidence, which was published in the January 2020 issue of Finance Research Letters, help answer the question of whether negative interest rates would lead to investors taking on more risk. They recruited 316 students from an undergraduate business lecture at a German university for an online experiment (participation was voluntary). German interest rates, both short- and longer-term, have been negative for a while.